1 DER BEGRIFF UMWELT
Inhalt:
1.0 Übersicht
1.1 Begriffsentwicklung
und Definitionen
1.2 Arten und
Bereiche von Umwelt
1.3 Lese-Tips
Info-Box
zu diesem Kapitel:
ARTEN VON UMWELT
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
natuerliche \ ¦
physische
<
>
Objekte oder Emissionen ¦ Wohn-U
/
gebaute /
¦
Arbeits-U
Umwelt
¦ Verkehrs-U
\
Individuen
(Nachbarn)
¦
Freizeit-U
soziale
<
¦ Versorgungs-U
Gruppe/Masse
¦
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
2 GEGENSTANDSBESTIMMUNG
UMWELT-/ÖKO-PSY
Inhalt:
2.0 Übersicht
2.1 Etiketten
und Definitionen
2.2 Forschungsfelder
2.3 Geschichte
der Ökopsychologie
2.4 Interdisziplinäre
Bezüge
2.5 Lese-Tips
Info-Box
zu diesem Kapitel:
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
3 ÖKOPSYCHOLOGISCHE
PERSPEKTIVEN
Inhalt:
3.0 Übersicht
3.1 Ökologische
Grundbegriffe
3.2 Mensch-Umwelt-Interaktionsmodelle
3.3 Theoretische
Ansätze
3.4 Lese-Tips
Einige
Info-Boxen zu diesem Kapitel:
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
4 UMWELTKOGNITIONEN
Inhalt:
4.0 Übersicht
4.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
4.2 Orientierung
in der Umwelt
4.3 Ästhetische
Empfindungen
4.4 Bewertung
der Umweltqualität
4.5 Lese-Tips
Einige
Info-Boxen zu diesem Kapitel:
BRUNSWIK'S "LENS
MODEL" OF ENVIRONMENTAL
PERCEPTION
(from Gifford 1997)
EIN TEXT ZUR "MOND-ILLUSION
Moon
sliver, wider than a mile - Why does the moon appear different sizes?
By Sandra Blaksee
<New York Times>
One of the most familiar illusions embraced
by the human brain has finally been solved, say a father-son pair of
scientists
who were armed with a contraption than projects fake moons onto the sky.
In the "moon illusion", the moon
rising or setting over the horizon appears much bigger than the moon
directly
overhead, even though both of the moons are the same size. By allowing viewers to move simulated moons
towards and away from each other, the scientists demonstrate that the
so-called
moon illusion seems to be caused by the brain's inability to estimate
huge
distances in the empty night sky. The
brain has many similar perceptual difficulties, they said, which
produce a
variety of convincing optical illusions.
An article explaining the moon illusion
appears in the 4 January issue of Proceeding
of the National Academy of Sciences, written by Dr. Lloyd Kaufman,
a professor
emeritus of psychology from New York University, and Dr. James Kaufman,
a
physicist at IMB's Almaden Research
Centre in San Jose, California.
The Kaufmans said the moon illusion had
baffled scientists since antiquity.
Aristotle, Ptolemy, Leonardo da Vinci, Johannes Kepler, Rene
Descartes
and many others had tried to explain it.
But now, scientists are able to take advantage of modern
insights into
the nature of human perception. After an
image falls on the retina, where it resembles the image on a camera
lens, it is
sent into the brain for further processing.
Within the brain's serpentine circuitry, every two-dimensional
image is
converted into a richer, more accurate three-dimensional representation
of the
world. It was within these many processing
steps that illusions were born, the scientists said.
"If you look at a photograph of a
person stretched out in front of you on the grass, you see enormous
feet and a
tiny head," Dr. Lloyd Kaufman said.
But the brain took that same image and made all sorts of
so-called size
constancy corrections so that the person did not look distorted. The visual "fact" that the person's
body was not misshapen was actually an illusion. Given the
understanding that
the human brain constantly deals in illusions, two theories have
emerged to
explain the moon illusion. They are
based on opposing views of how the perceptual system computes size and
distance.
One theory attributes the moon illusion to
how the brain deals with the apparent size of distant objects. Because of how the light falls on the retina,
and other details, the brain judges the moon to be smaller than it
really is,
and thus father away, when it is viewed in empty space.
The second theory holds that the moon
illusion arises from how the brain deals with apparent distance not
size. When people view the moon on the
horizon, the
story goes, their brains use myriad signals from foreground terrain to
compute
the moon's size. Even in flat terrain,
the horizon appears very far away, so any large object seen there, like
a
mountain must be enormous.
Thus the moon is treated like a mountain -
Huge and very far away. But when people
look straight up at the moon, they no longer have land clues to compute
distances. Their brains- and not their
eyes - fail to adjust to the enormous distance involved and perceive
the moon
as being closer and closer as it rises into the night sky.
The brain interprets this apparent distance
discrepancy to mean that the overhead moon is much smaller than it is.
To find out which theory is correct, the
Kaufmans invented a way to measure how people perceive distances to the
moon. Virtually all other experiments
had dealt with how people perceived the size of the moon, the Kaufmans
said.
Their device consists of a laptop computer,
a mirror and two lenses that can project luminous discs, or simulated
moons,
into the cloudless daytime sky. The
apparatus works like a stereogram.
Double images are directed into each eye, where they fuse
somewhere
inside the brain, giving the illusion of depth.
Dr Lloyd Kaufman took five people to a
hillside and had them look through the apparatus until they saw two
identical
moons side by side. One was a fixed moon
that they could hold steady. The other
was a reference moon that they could move forward and back in space. People were asked to position the reference
moon so it was halfway between themselves and fixed moon, the Kaufmans
said.
When moving the reference moon in relation
to a fixed moon on the horizon, people place the reference moon about
33 metres
from themselves, the scientists said.
But, when they moved the reference moon in relation to an
overhead moon,
they placed it only eight metres away.
Thus, the "halfway" point to a horizon moon was four times
father away than the halfway point to an overhead moon.
In a second experiment, subjects looked at
two moons projected high overhead and pressing a key, moved one closer
to
themselves. They were startled to find
that as the moving moon got closer, it always appeared to be getting
smaller and
smaller, not bigger.
The two experiments confirm the second
theory, the Kaufmans said. If the brain
were attending to size, the closer-in moon should appear larger, but it
did
not.
Source: New York Times;
cited
from The Age, 24-01-00
TEXT ZU MOND-PHASEN UND VERHALTEN
A
Recycled Cycle: Moon Phases and Behaviour
Folklore and commonly held beliefs maintain
that many aspects of our behaviour are related to phases of the moon. Sexual prowess, menstrual cycles, birthrates,
death rates, suicide rates, homicide rates, and hospital admission
rates are
among the phenomena various people claim are affected by the moon. Often, it is maintained that a full moon
increases strange behaviour. Surveys of
undergraduates indicate that half of them believe people behave
strangely when
the moon is full (Rotton & Kelly, 1995a).
Other beliefs are that the tidal pulls of full and new moons
influence
human physiology or psychic functioning, or that the moon's perigee
(closest
distance to the Earth) and apogee (farthest distance from the Earth)
influence
us in strange ways. Indeed, the word lunacy is derived from a belief in a
relationship between the moon and mental illness.
The last full moon of the previous
millennium occurred on December 22, 1999, which also corresponded to
its
perigee, as well as to the winter solstice or shortest day in the
Northern
Hemisphere. In addition, the moon on
that date was almost as close to the sun as it ever gets.
As a result, it appeared 14% larger than at
its apogee as well as 7% brighter. No
particular anomalies in human behaviour were reported.
But the lore says that when a similar full
moon, perigee, and solstice occurred on December 20 and 21, 1866, the
Sioux
warrior Crazy Horse was inspired to ambush and wipe out 80 U.S.
soldiers
(Golden, 1999).
It is the case that lunar tides affect some
marine organisms, and there is some evidence a full moon ever so
slightly
increases temperatures on Earth (by 0.02° K; Balling & Cerveny,
1995). From time to time, research also
appears that
actually gives credence to beliefs that the moon causes drastic changes
in
human behaviour. For example, Blackman
and Catalina (1973) found that full moons were associated with an
increase in
the number of patients visiting a psychiatric emergency room. In another study, Lieber and Sherin (1972)
reported a relationship between moon phase and homicide.
Rape, robbery, and assault; burglary,
larceny, and theft; and auto theft, drunkenness, disorderly conduct,
and
attacks on family and children have also been linked to a full moon
(Tasso
& Miller, 1976). At first glance,
then, it would appear that science has confirmed the folklore of the
ancients
(see also Garzino, 1982).
Not so fast! Closer
examination of the data indicates that
the mysticism of the lunar cycle may be more myth than reality. Campbell (1982), Kelly, Rotton, and Culver
(1985-86), and Rotton and Kelly (1985b, 1987), among others, have
reviewed the
available research on the topic and concluded that no firm relationship
exists
between any lunar variable and human behaviour (see also Byrnes &
Kelly,
1992). For example, studies conducted
over a period of three to five years may report a relationship between
the full
moon and suicide or homicide for only one of the years studied. Researchers who conclude that such a
relationship exists are ignoring the fact that it does not exist for
the other
years, or that these behaviours are actually lower during full moons
for
another year. Moreover, it is
consistently found that crimes increase on weekends.
For some periods of the year, lunar phases
may coincide with weekends. Data based
on only these periods will obviously show a relationship between the
moon and crime,
but data based on other periods will show the opposite relationship or
no
relationship at all. In addition, a
self-fulfilling prophecy may operate: If police believe crime increases
during
a full moon, they may become more vigilant at these times and thus
arrest more
people. Altogether, the evidence
suggests that positive links between moon phases and behaviour are
spurious and
are attributable to mere chance probabilities in the data or to
variables not
considered by individual investigators.
Why do these mistaken beliefs persist?
Reasons include misconceptions about physical processes (Culver,
Rotton,
& Kelly, 1988), attitudes acquired from one's peers (Rotton, Kelly,
&
Elortegui, 1986), and cognitive biases, such as basing conclusions on
only a
few occurrences. Given the tenacity of
beliefs in moon phases causing disruptive behaviour, we suspect the
lunacy of
it all will continue for some time!
Source: Bell, P.A., Greene, T.C., Fisher, J.D.,
& Baum, A. (2001).
Environmental Psychology.
Orlando, FL: Harcourt .
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
5 RÄUMLICHES
UMWELTVERHALTEN
Inhalt:
5.0 Übersicht
5.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
5.2 Persönliches
Raum- und Distanz-Verhalten
5.3 Effekte
räumlicher/sozialer Dichte
5.4 Umzugsverhalten
und Wohnungswahl
5.5 Lese-Tips
Einige
Info-Boxen zu diesem Kapitel:
DAS ENTSCHEIDUNGSPROBLEM
"UMZUG"
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
> Entscheidungen:
o Bleiben oder
Umziehen?
o {falls Umzug:} Wechsel von
Wohnung/Haus/Ortsteil/Stadt/Region/Land?
> Prozesse
bei der Wohnungswahl
o
Suchen
o
Besichtigen
o
Bewerten
o
Wählen
> Umziehen
= räumliches Verhalten auf
Makro-Ebene
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
=> Vgl. Modell in Materialien zu Kap. 9 !
CONTACT
& INTERACTION ZONES
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
6 Exkurs
1: LEHRFILM "KONTAKT UND DISTANZ"
Inhalt:
6.0 Einleitung
6.1 F i l m
6.2 Diskussion
Info-Box
zu diesem Kapitel:
:: This film was made at
Frankfurt University
:: many years ago - - yet it
is still instructive
::
for
lectures in Environmental Psychology
Lese-Tips
Autoren:
PREISER & WANNENMACHER 1980
7 NUTZUNG
VON
UMWELTEN
Inhalt:
7.0 Übersicht
7.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
7.2 Nutzung
und Nutzbarkeit
7.3 Untersuchung
von Nutzerverhalten
7.4 Lese-Tips
Eine
Info-Box zu diesem Kapitel:
THEMEN-UEBERSICHT ZU
NUTZER-ZIELE,
NUTZUNGSVERHALTEN, NUTZBARKEIT
Relevante Aktivitaetsbereiche:
o Arbeiten o
Wohnen o
Erholen
Nutzerverhalten
o Sich orientieren o
Aktivitäten durchführen
o Umwelt verändern
Nutzbarkeits-Aspekte:
o Funktionalität o
Raumbedarf o Ortswechsel
Untersuchung von Nutzerverhalten:
Analyse des raum-zeitl. Verhaltens:
o Orte Zeiten
o Wege
Methodik:
o Beobachten o
Befragen
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
8 UMWELTSTRESSOREN
UND IHRE AUSWIRKUNGEN
Inhalt:
8.0 Übersicht
8.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
8.2 Arten und
Auftreten von Umweltstressoren
8.3 Beeinträchtigende
Wirkungen
8.4 Bevölkerungsreaktionen
8.5 Minderungsmaßnahmen
8.6 Lese-Tips
Einige
Info-Boxen zu diesem Kapitel:
DECIBEL[A] SCALE AND NOISE
EXAMPLES
CURVES OF EQUAL PERCEIVED
LOUDNESS FOR dB AND Hz LEVELS
HUMAN EAR
INNER EAR
THE INFLUENCE OF BACKGROUND
NOISE AND DISTANCE BETWEEN COMMUNICATORS
ON COMMUNICATION FEASIBILITY
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
9 Exkurs
2: BEISPIEL FORSCHUNGSPROJEKT
Inhalt:
9.0 Übersicht
9.1 Problemstellung
9.2 Untersuchungsansatz
9.3 Datenerhebung
9.4 Einige
Ergebnisse
9.5 Anwendung
der Befunde
9.6 Lese-Tips
Eine
Info-Box zu diesem Kapitel:
A
MODEL FOR ANALYZING RESIDENTIAL CHOICE
Lese-Tips
Rohrmann, B., & Borcherding,
K. (1992). Urteils- und Entscheidungsprozesse zur Wohnumwelt. In K.
Pawlik
& K. H. Stapf (Eds.), Umwelt und
Verhalten (217-245). Bern: Huber.
10 GESTALTUNG
VON UMWELT
Inhalt:
10.0 Übersicht
10.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
10.2 Zielkriterien
10.3 Angriffspunkte
der Umweltgestaltung
10.4 Der
Planungsprozess
10.5 Partizipation
von Beteiligten
10.6 Lese-Tips
Einige
Info-Boxen zu diesem Kapitel:
WICHTIGE
GESTALTUNGSMITTEL
ZU DENEN
UMWELTPSYCHOLOGISCHE EXPERTISE VORLIEGT
Formgebung
Materialwahl
Farbe
Räumliche
Anordnung
Wegeführung
Licht, Kilma, Schall, ...
Entsorgung
Orientierungshinweise
Interaktionsräume
Begrünung
SOCIOPETAL AND SOCIOFUGAL FURNITURE ARRANGEMENTS
PLANUNGS-MODELL (nach Fischer 1995)
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
11 SPEZIELLE
BERUFLICHE UND PRIVATE UMWELTEN
Inhalt:
11.0 Übersicht
11.1 Arten
baulicher Umwelten
11.2 Wohnungen/Häuser/Siedlungen
11.3 Anforderungen
& Probleme: Öffentliche
Bauten
11.4 Natur -
Gestaltung und Verwendung
11.5 Sozialwissenschaftliche
Lösungsbeiträge
11.6 Lese-Tips
Eine
Info-Box zu diesem Kapitel:
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
12 ÖKOLOGISCHE
ATTITÜDEN
Inhalt:
12.0 Übersicht
12.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
12.2 Dimensionen
ökologischer Attitüden
12.3 Beeinflussung
von Umweltattitüden
12.4 Lese-Tips
Eine
Info-Box zu diesem Kapitel:
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
13 UMWELTSCHUTZVERHALTEN
Inhalt:
13.0 Übersicht
13.1 Bereiche
von Umweltverhalten
13.2 Motivierung
zu individuellem Umweltschutz
13.3 Gesellschaftlicher
Kontext & ökologische
Bewegung
13.4 Lese-Tips
Einige
Info-Boxen zu diesem Kapitel:
Motivierung zu
individuellem Umweltschutz
Einflussfaktoren:
o Attitüden
o Wissen
o Subjektive
Wertigkeit
o Gewohnheiten
o Verhaltensanreize
=> reinforcer:
Lob, Geld, Privilegien, Memos, Feedback;
=> model
behaviour:
Attraktive
und praktische Möglichkeiten, vorhandene Vorbilder, positiver
Ausgangszustand
GROB (1995):
THEORETICAL MODEL OF ENVIRONMENTAL BEHAVIOR
GROB (1995): EMPIRICAL
STRUCTURAL MODEL
OF ENVIRONMENTAL BEHAVIOR
A NOTE ON
PRINCIPAL DILEMMAS
IN THE MANAGEMENT
OF ENVIRONMENTAL PROBLEMS
The responsible use of resources has
been debated for a long time, yet problem solutions are difficult
because of
conflicting interests and needs. Such
problems can be analyzed in terms of underlying decision
dilemmas, as especially social psychologists have pointed
out.
For example, the fuels that fill
most of our current energy needs (oil, coal, and natural gas) are in
short
supply and dwindling rapidly. Other
resources such as water, trees and metals also will become scarce in
the near
future if present trends continue. We
all know this, yet little changes in our day to day behaviour. Why do people knowingly persist in what is
ultimately self-destructive behaviour?
Our inability to manage natural
resources efficiently may be traced to what Hardin (1968) called the tragedy of the commons. Originally,
the commons referred to public
land areas on which anyone could graze livestock with no cost to
themselves. The tendency was to exploit
the commons by grazing one's sheep on the public land, thereby
preserving
resources on one's own land. The tragedy
lies in the fact that the commons is a limited resource used by
individuals
acting in there own self-interest. Such
a community consumes resources at a rate that endangers the very
existence. Such a community consumes
resources at a rate that endangers the very existence of the resources
itself. The story of the commons is an
apt metaphor for all the problems of limited resources that we now face.
The conflict between individual and
group interests is called a commons
dilemma. It is exacerbated by the
tendency to choose immediate rewards even though they have serious
long-term
costs, which is a decision also referred to as social trap. Dawes (1980)
proposed the more general term social
dilemma to encompass all these maladaptive, resource-related
behaviours.
A valuable elaboration was provided
by Vlek & Keren (1992) who systematically analyze
types of dilemmas in environmental risk
management (influenced by decision theory).
The general
problem is the benefit/risk dilemma:
These are
situations in which benefits are not available without significant
risks.
(Problem: the more people are eager to get hold of benefits the more
likely
will they neglect to appropriately weigh the risks).
Three specific
dilemmas are:
Temporal dilemma (or time trap):
Problem: Long-term
risks tend to get less weight than immediate benefits.
Social traps:
Problem:
Individual interests are often incompatible with societal interests.
Spatial dilemma:
Problem:
Decision-makers may not give much weight
to risks for groups or nations living elsewhere.
Careless littering or polluting is a
good example for what's sometimes called the "individual-good/collective-bad
trap" - that is,
short-term inconvenience outweighs the long-term benefits of an
aesthetically
pleasing and healthy environment. (In
this case social traps are intertwined with temporal traps).
Most of the empirical research on
human behaviour in social dilemmas has been done in the laboratory
using
simulated games to observe how people manage common resources. Edney's (1979) "Nuts Game" is a
good example of such a simulation. In
the game, three or more subjects sit around a shallow, nonbreakable,
open bowl
containing ten hardware hexagonal nuts.
Each player's goal is to get as many nuts as possible. Players can take nuts from the bowl at
anytime after the game starts, knowing that the experimenter will
double the
number of nuts remaining in the bowl after each ten-second interval. The game continues until some time limit is
reached or the bowl is empty. The wise
strategy, of course, is to show restraint - take one or two nuts out
during
each period, and gradually accumulate a stockpile.
Edney reports, however, that about two thirds
of the groups never even make it to the first replenishment stage! Usually a greedy frenzy of grabbing instantly
destroys what would otherwise be a self-replenishing resource.
According to
Edney, the nuts in the bowl symbolise any limited resource pool (Whales
or oil,
for example). In spite of the fact that
people know the limits of the resources, social pressures in the
situation
encourage resource-destructive behaviour.
Source:
The above was compiled by BR, using
material from Dawes 1980,
McAndrew 1993; Vaughan & Hogg 1995, Vlek & Keren 1992.
A NOTE ON THE
COMPLEXITY OF SAVING-THE-ENVIRONMENT ACTIONS
[from
Bell et al. 2001, Environmental
Psychology, p. 472]
"Actions
to
"save" the environment often involve trade-offs. Actions with low
impact on one segment of the environment often have high impact on
another
segment. For example, using paper plates to save water costs trees;
using
washable dishes saves trees but costs water and places a burden on
waste
treatment facilities. A (controversial) case can actually be made that
plastic
cups have less impact on the environment than paper cups. The more we
wash
dishes and diapers, the more we place burdens on wastewater facilities
sucha s
this one."
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
14 Exkurs
3: EINSCHÄTZUNG UMWELTRISIKEN
Inhalt:
14.0 Übersicht
14.1 Forschungsfeld
14.2 Psychologische
Ansaetze
14.3 Kognitive
Struktur von Risikourteilen
14.4..Risiko-Information
und -Kommunikation
14.5 Lese-Tips
Info-Boxen
zu diesem Kapitel:
Source: Rohrmann 2003
:: For further material about
socio-psychological
:: risk research see
www.rohrmannresearch.net,
:: click "Project
RPX" and "Project RAC" there.
Lese-Tips
Burton, I., Kates,
R.W., White, G. F., (1993). The
environment as hazard. New York: Guilford Press.
Jungermann, H., Rohrmann, B., & Wiedemann, P.
(1991). Risikokontroversen - Konzepte,
Konflikte, Kommunikation. Berlin etc.: Springer.
Rohrmann, B. (1994). Risk perception of different
societal groups: Australian findings and cross-national comparisons. Australian Journal of Psychology, 46,
150-163.
Rohrmann, B. (2003). Perception of risk:
Research, results, relevance.
In J. Gough (Ed.), Sharing the future - Risk communication in
practice
(pp. 21-44). Christchurch: CAE, University of Canterbury.
Slovic, P. (1992). Perception of risk: Reflections
on
the psychometric paradigm. In D. Golding & S. Krimsky (Eds.), Theories of risk (117-139). London:
Praeger.
15 METHODIK
UMWELTPSYCHOLOGISCHER FORSCHUNG
Inhalt:
15.0 Übersicht
15.1 Relevante
Untersuchungsansätze
15.2 Spezielle
Datenerhebungsverfahren
15.3 Validität
von Befunden
15.4 Lese-Tips
Eine
Info-Box zu diesem Kapitel:
GRAPH re TYPES OF
EMPIRICAL RESEARCH
(Rohrmann 2000)
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
16 UMWELT-/ÖKO-PSYCHOLOGIE
ALS BERUFSFELD
Inhalt:
16.0 Übersicht
16.1 Erkenntnisbedarf
16.2 Methodologie
umweltpsychologischer Expertise
16.3 Anwendung
und Anwendbarkeit
16.4 Möglichkeiten
beruflicher Tätigkeit
16.5 Lese-Tips
Einige
Info-Boxen zu diesem Kapitel:
PROBLEMS IN APPLYING
ENVIRONMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCH
ACADEMIC RESEARCHERS - DIFFICULTIES
IN BEING PRACTICAL?
EPILOG
IN THE ENVIRONMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY TEXTBOOK BY ROBERT GIFFORD
"Environmental Psychology - The
essential and challenging discipline"
(2003)
The
first
fundamental truth of environmental psychology is that every
human activity occurs in a physical context. Whether one is
interested in environmental psychology itself, in another area of
psychology,
or even in a different discipline, all the action occurs in offices,
labs,
outdoors, studios, residences, vehicles, institutions, or factories,
and all
these settings affect everyone's behavior and well‑being. The second
fundamental truth is that mutual influence of person and setting
usually is
neither sudden nor dramatic (certain natural disasters are exceptions).
Because
these influences are often cumulative and subtle and because we often
adapt
even to bad settings, they often go unnoticed.
Because
of
these two truths, the vital importance of person‑environment
transactions
sometimes is overlooked. But closer inspection of reality and
reflection on the
previous fifteen chapters forces one to realize that environmental
psychology
is an essential human endeavor. You may already agree, but it is to
remember
that not everyone else did, or does.
In
its
early days, psychology focused on mental dynamics. Later it began to
realize
the importance of interpersonal and social forces. Except for the
lonely voices
of a few pioneers, the role played by the physical environment was only
recognized 35 years ago. Even today, if your experiences are like mine,
you
must often field questions about what environmental psychology is.
Someone
hears that you are studying it and they say, "That sounds interesting.
But
what is it?" In fact, on this very day, my barber asked me that
question!
Try to take great care in giving an answer, because the public at large
still
is generally unaware of this field; its opinion about environmental
psychology
depends in part on what it hears from you and me.
Of
course,
you must develop your own assessment of the field to offer the curious.
Years
of work have convinced me that environmental psychologists have bravely
undertaken a task that is ultimately more difficult than that
undertaken by any
other branch of science. This book never offered to provide simple
recipes for
solutions to practical problems, and I doubt there are any simple
answers to
the practical and theoretical problems that environmental psychologists
try to
solve. Although knowledge is growing so fast that it fairly bulges out
of the
book, it still provides illumination of theoretical issues and broad
research
questions more like that offered by the full moon than that offered by
the
noonday sun.
At
this
stage, my hope is that readers have learned the real strengths of
environmental
psychology: having the courage to struggle with complex problems, and
refining
the methods needed to understand and overcome these problems in local
community
settings. At present, reading about research and theory in
environmental
psychology requires more tolerance for uncertainty than a willingness
to master
a set of established laws. However, the techniques and procedures
needed to
conduct applied research that can significantly improve the
habitability of
built and natural settings in your own local community are already well
developed
and ready for use.
This
is all
that may reasonably be expected, in my view, after only 35 years of
sustained
effort. Give us another few decades and resources like those allocated
to other
areas of science and the growth of understanding of the individual,
social and
societal processes involved in environmental psychology will compare
very
favorably with progress in older disciplines. One reason for this lies
in the
degree of dedication of most environmental psychologists who, in my
experience,
are even more driven than most others in the knowledge business. They
seem to
derive extra energy from knowing their work forms an integral part of
their
everyday life; that they can affect the world around them; that they
are always
in a physical setting that affects them, is affected by them, and might
be
improved by their efforts.
Lese-Tips
{{ folgen spaeter }}
>>> Umweltpsychologie
Endgültige
Gliederung der Vorlesung von
Prof. Rohrmann (2008)
1 DER
BEGRIFF UMWELT
1.0 Übersicht
1.1 Begriffsentwicklung
und Definitionen
1.2 Arten
und Bereiche von Umwelt
1.3 Lese-Tips
2 GEGENSTANDSBESTIMMUNG
UMWELT-/ÖKO-PSYCHOLOGIE
2.0 Übersicht
2.1 Etiketten
und Definitionen
2.2 Forschungsfelder
2.3 Geschichte
der Umwelt-/Ökopsychologie
2.4 Interdisziplinäre
Bezüge
2.5 Lese-Tips
3 ÖKOPSYCHOLOGISCHE
PERSPEKTIVEN
3.0 Übersicht
3.1 Ökologische
Grundbegriffe
3.2 Mensch-Umwelt-Interaktionsmodelle
3.3 Theoretische
Ansätze zur Reaktion auf
Umweltbedingungen
3.4 Lese-Tips
4 UMWELTKOGNITIONEN
4.0 Übersicht
4.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
4.2 Orientierung
in der Umwelt
4.E Exkurs:
Erstellung eigener 'cognitive maps'
4.3 Ästhetische
Empfindungen
4.4 Bewertung
der Umweltqualität
4.5 Lese-Tips
5 RÄUMLICHES
UMWELTVERHALTEN
5.0 Übersicht
5.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
5.2 Persönliches
Raum- und Distanz-Verhalten
5.3 Effekte
räumlicher/sozialer Dichte
5.4 Umzugsverhalten
und Wohnungswahl {gekürzt}
5.5 Lese-Tips
6 Exkurs
1: LEHRFILM "KONTAKT UND DISTANZ"
6.0 Einleitung
6.1 F
i l m
6.2 Diskussion
7 NUTZUNG
VON UMWELTEN
7.0 Übersicht
7.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
7.2 Nutzung
und Nutzbarkeit
7.3 Untersuchung
von Nutzerverhalten {gekürzt}
7.4 Lese-Tips
8 UMWELTSTRESSOREN
UND IHRE AUSWIRKUNGEN
8.0 Übersicht
8.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
8.2 Arten
und Auftreten von Umweltstressoren
8.3 Beeinträchtigende
Wirkungen
8.4 Bevölkerungsreaktionen {gekürzt}
8.5 Minderungsmaßnahmen
8.6 Lese-Tips
9 Exkurs
2: BEISPIEL EINES FORSCHUNGSPROJEKTES
-- Thema: Wohnungswahl
9.4 Einige
Ergebnisse {die
anderen Abschnitte wurden wegen
Zeitmangel gestrichen}
10 GESTALTUNG
VON UMWELT
10.0 Übersicht
10.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
10.2 Zielkriterien
10.3 Angriffspunkte
der Umweltgestaltung
10.4 Der
Planungsprozess
10.5 Partizipation
von Beteiligten
10.6 Lese-Tips
11 SPEZIELLE
BERUFLICHE UND PRIVATE UMWELTEN
11.0 Übersicht
11.1 Arten
baulicher Umwelten
11.2 Wohnungen/Häuser/Siedlungen
11.3 Anforderungen
& Probleme: Öffentliche
Bauten/Einrichtungen {gekürzt}
11.4 Natur
- Gestaltung und Verwendung
11.5 Sozialwissenschaftliche
Lösungsbeiträge
11.E Exkurs:
Zeichentrickfilm aus Melbourne über klassische
Stadtgestaltung
11.6 Lese-Tips
12 ÖKOLOGISCHE
ATTITÜDEN
12.0 Übersicht
12.1 Wesentliche
Begriffe
12.2 Dimensionen
ökologischer Attitüden
12.3 Beeinflussung
von Umweltattitüden
12.4 Lese-Tips
13 UMWELTSCHUTZVERHALTEN
13.0 Übersicht
13.1 Bereiche
von Umweltverhalten
13.2 Motivierung
zu individuellem Umweltschutz
13.3 Gesellschaftlicher/politischer
Kontext und
ökologische Bewegung {gekürzt}
13.4 Lese-Tips
14 Exkurs
3: EINSCHÄTZUNG UND BEWERTUNG VON UMWELTRISIKEN
14.0 Übersicht
14.1 Forschungsfeld
14.2 Psychologische
Ansaetze
14.3 Kognitive
Struktur von Risikourteilen
14.4 Risiko-Information
und -Kommunikation
14.5 Lese-Tips
15 METHODIK
UMWELTPSYCHOLOGISCHER FORSCHUNG
15.0 Übersicht
15.1 Relevante
Untersuchungsansätze
15.2 Spezielle
Datenerhebungsverfahren {gekürzt}
15.3 Forschung
mit Umwelt-Simulationen
15.E Experiment:
Evaluation von Computerbildern einer Universitäts-Umwelt
15.4 Validität
von Befunden
15.5 Lese-Tips
16 UMWELT-/ÖKO-PSYCHOLOGIE
ALS BERUFSFELD
16.0 Übersicht
16.1 Erkenntnisbedarf
16.2 Methodologie
umweltpsychologischer Expertise
16.3 Anwendung
und Anwendbarkeit
16.4 Möglichkeiten
beruflicher Tätigkeit
16.5 Lese-Tips
&&
Präsentation
des Buchs "DIE STADT" vn Hermann HESSE (1910)
illustriert mit Zeichnungen von Walter
SCHMÖGNER
<<< ENDE DER
UPI-MATERIALIEN >>>